Effect Size

"If you torture the data long enough, it will confess." Ronald Coase.

The effect size (ES) statistic is the cornerstone of Hattie's work. He claimed that the larger the ES the greater the impact, "know thy impact", on student learning and this enabled him to rank educational influences and list the top-ranked influences as "what works best".

However, in 2018, in an interview with Ollie Lovell, he admitted his rankings were misleading and that he does not rank anymore - listen @ 1hr 21minutes here. 

Example of an Effect Size Calculation

Clark & Mayer (2016) give a simple example,

The control group received a basic multimedia lesson that explained content with graphics and audio narration. We call this the no-music group. The experimental group received the same lesson with background music added to the narration. We call this the music group. 

Suppose the no-music group averaged 90 percent correct on a test of the material and the music group averaged 80 percent on the same test. 

Also suppose the scores were not very spread out, so most of the no-music students scored close to 90 and most of the music students scored close to 80. 

The standard deviation tells you how spread out the scores are, or how much variation there is in the results.

Clark & Mayer (2016) provide the following summary and Effect Size calculation.



The ES is equivalent to a 'Z-score' of a standard normal distribution. For example, an effect size of 1 means that the score of the average person in the experimental (treatment) group is 1 standard deviation above the average person in the control group (no treatment).

Note: In the above simple example the Standard Deviation (SD) of both groups of students is 10. In reality these two SD are rarely the same. So another important aspect is how to deal with 2 different SD - see the section on SD below.

There are many OTHER ways to calculate an Effect Size.

Hattie reports that these are the 2 major ways (VL, p. 8):


Problem 1 - Can You Directly Compare Effect Sizes Calculated by Different Methods?

The peer review below shows there is significant doubt about Hattie's foundational premise that ES from disparate studies can be compared to determine "what works best".

E.g., Bergeron & Rivard (2017) warn,
"These two types of effects are not equivalent and cannot be directly compared... A statistician would already be asking many questions and would have an enormous doubt towards the entire methodology in Visible Learning and its derivatives."
Hattie has finally admitted (in a small footnote) that this is an issue, in Hattie & Hamilton (2020) they concede,
"Note that there are two methods for calculating effect size (pre-post and intervention comparison) and they can lead to different interpretations." (footnote, p. 4)
However, Hattie's inconsistency is concerning as he continues to compare studies using different methods of ES calculation and make bold claims with his commercial partner Corwin - see Hattie's Presentations.

Note that Hattie also continues to use a 3rd method of converting a correlation to an ES. 

Significantly, Hattie's highest ES has been derived from the poorer quality correlation studies, e.g., Collective Teacher Efficacy, Self-Report Grades and Piagetian Programs.

The 3rd Method - Correlation Converted to an Effect size

Despite Hattie's claim of using the above 2 methods to calculate ES, several scholars, Blatchford (2016), Bergeron & Rivard (2017), Wrigley (2018), Bakker et al. (2019) & Kraft (2020) have identified that Hattie most often uses a 3rd method of converting a correlation to an ES and there are major problems with this - see Correlation.

The 2 largest evidence-based educational organizations, the USAs What Works Clearing House (WWC), and the UKs Education Endowment Foundation (EEF), do NOT consider correlational studies to be of high enough quality to include them in their toolkits for teachers.

There are Even More Ways to Calculate the ES!

Simpson (2017) explains, 
"...while calculating an effect size may be simple enough for a first course in statistics, there are considerable subtleties in understanding it sufficiently well to ensure that the processes of combining effect sizes in meta-analyses allow valid conclusions to be drawn." (p. 452)
There are further variations in the ES calculation that different researchers use, e.g., Cohen's d, Hedges' g and Glass’s Δ. Each of these methods uses a different Standard Deviation. 

This creates more problems when comparing studies - see section SD below.

This is best summarised by a report from John Mandrola,
"The Year’s Most Important Study Adds to Uncertainty in Science."
Mandrola summarises a large study by Nosek et al (2018) who recruited 29 teams comprising 61 researchers to use the SAME data, but came up with 29 totally DIFFERENT effect sizes!

Randomisation Also Greatly Influences the ES

To remove the effect of confounding variables, students are randomly assigned to a control & experimental group. Note the medical method also insists on "double blindness". That is, neither the control or experimental group nor the staff know, who is getting the treatment.

Cheung & Slavin (2016) support the concern of which method to use to calculate the ES,
"...effect sizes are significantly higher in quasi-experiments than in randomized experiments."
Slavin (2015) details the difference,
"Matched quasi-experiments did produce inflated effect sizes (ES=+0.23 for quasi-experiments, +0.16 for randomized). This difference is not nearly as large as other factors we looked at, such as sample size (small studies greatly exaggerate outcomes), use of experimenter-made measures, and published vs. unpublished sources (experimenter-made tests and published sources exaggerate impacts). But our findings about matched vs. randomized studies are reason for caution about putting too much faith in quasi-experiments."
Berk (2011) concurs with Slavin and Bergeron & Rivard,
"when the studies are not randomized experiments, there is a strong likelihood that a collection of biased treatment effect estimates is being combined. How is one then better off? Biased estimates are not random errors and do not cancel out. The result can be just a more precise causal estimate that has the wrong sign and is systematically far too large or far too small." (p. 199)
Further analyses of RCT Evans & Yuan (2022) found a median effect size of 0.07 and Lortie-Forgues & Inglis (2019) also found a low average effect size of 0.06 with a large confidence width of 0.30.

Many scholars are critical of Hattie's lack of quality control, e.g., 

Hattie cites many meta-analyses from Slavin, but Slavin (2017), is very critical of Hattie's method,
"Hattie includes literally everything in his meta-meta analyses, including studies with no control groups, studies in which the control group never saw the content assessed by the post-test, and so on."
Slavin (2017) explains the use of some sort of quality control would remove,  
"...a lot of the awful research that gives Hattie the false impression that everything works, and fabulously."
As a result of all these issues, Slavin (2018) posted that John Hattie is Wrong!

Hattie's lack of quality control, in part, explains the vast difference in the conclusions of Hattie compared to EEF, WWC and others - see Other Researchers.

Problem 2. Student Achievement is measured in different ways or not at all - A VALIDITY problem!

Some examples of different tests used:

Standardized tests, specific tests, physical tests (the number of times a ball can be caught off the wall),  a parent's rating of their child out of 5, IQ and many examples of measuring something else like hyperactivity & engagement.

Hattie claims all of the studies he used focused on student achievement. But this is clearly NOT the case as many studies measured something else like hyperactivity (see- Behavior).

Wecker et al. (2017, p. 28) confirm this saying, Hattie mistakenly included studies that do not measure academic performance.

Fletcher-Wood (2021) commenting on Hattie's prime Feedback study,
"Kluger and DeNisi focused on the way feedback affects behaviour – not how it affects learning."
Even if all the studies did measure Student Achievement, there is a growing body of evidence showing that the test used can determine the ES, e.g., standardized tests generate lower ES than specific tests.

Simpson (2017, p. 461) gives examples of specific tests designed for a particular influence, e.g., improving algebra skills, resulting in a 40% higher ES than a standardized test on the same students!

Kraft (2019) confirms this,
"Even among measures of student achievement, effect sizes for researcher-designed and specialized topic tests aligned with the treatment are often two to four times larger than effects on broad standardized state tests (Lipsey et al., 2012; Cheung & Slavin, 2016)" (p. 8).
Simpson (2017, p. 462) details more problems with tests. He shows tests with more questions give 400% higher ES.

Simpson (2018b, p. 5) also shows problems with different standardized tests for the SAME maths intervention,
"The effect size... for the PIM test was 0.33 and for the SENT-R-B test was 1.11."
Slavin (2019) has also written extensively on this issue and confirms Simpson's analysis.

Hattie just ignores these issues.

So comparing these ES is the classic 'apples versus oranges' problem.

The page Student Achievement discusses in more detail the HUGE question of what is student achievement? There is NO consensus on what it is. So, how does one measure it???

Blichfeldt (2011),
"We also get no information about how 'learning outcomes' are defined or measured in the studies at different levels, what tests are used, which subjects are tested and how."
Many of the scholars that Hattie used also comment on this problem,

DuPaul & Eckert (2012),

"It is difficult to compare effect size estimates across research design types. Not only are effect size estimates calculated differently for each research design, but there appear to be differences in the types of outcome measures used across designs." (p. 408).
Kelley & Camilli (2007), 
"methodological variations across the studies make it problematic to draw coherent generalisations. These summaries illustrate the diversity in study characteristics including child samples, research designs, measurement, independent and dependent variables, and modes of analysis." (p. 7).
Problem 3. The Control Group (or lack of one):

Simpson (2017) & Bergeron & Rivard (2017) give examples of how the same influence, depending on how you define the control and experimental groups, can give ES ranging from 0 to infinity!

As a result, Bergeron & Rivard & Simpson call into question Hattie's entire use of ES comparisons, e.g., Simpson (2017),
"standardised effect size is a research tool for individual studies, not a policy tool for directing whole educational areas. These meta-meta-analyses which order areas on the basis of effect size are thus poor selection mechanisms for driving educational policy and should not be used for directing large portions of a country’s education budget." (p. 463)
Bakker et al. (2019) confirmed these problems (detailed below) with Hattie's work and concluded,
"...his lists of effect sizes ignore these points and are therefore misleading."
Also, Simpson (2017) details other problems,
"the experimental condition in some studies and meta-analyses is the comparison condition in others." (p. 455)
You can listen to podcasts - Bergeron (2018) and Simpson (2018p).

Many other scholars also warn of this problem,

Wrigley (2018also discusses the problem of control groups about Hattie's work,
"should the control group experience the absence of the practice being trialed, or simply 'business as usual'? 
This ambiguity concerning the control group can seriously distort attempts to calculate an 'effect size'.
We do not learn whether teachers and teaching assistants in the control group had any access to training comparable to that of the treatment group, whether they also taught small
classes, or what 'business as usual' actually involved" 
(p. 363).
"Sometimes Hattie uses 'effect size' to mean 'as compared to a control group' and at other times to mean 'as compared to the same students before the study started'" (p. 368).
Poulsen (2014) identifies that Hattie often uses studies that do not have control groups and concludes,
"...not much cannot be concluded about learning outcomes" (p. 3, translated from Danish).
Nielsen and Klitmøller (2017) concur,
"The meta-analyses... do not have uniform standards for, how they measure the effect. In many meta-analyses, studies involving the effect are not related to the use of control groups" (translated from Danish, p. 4).
Also, Lervåg &  Melby-Lervåg (2014),
"If you do not have a control group, the effect size will be calculated only on the basis of performance on the mapping before and after the action. The effect size will then be artificially high without this being a correct image. An example of this from Hattie's book is that vocabulary programs come out with a very high effect size."
Hattie's flagship study on Feedback, Kluger & DeNisi (1989), also warn generally of the problem of lack of control groups in educational studies,
"Without control groups, we may know more about the relative merits of several types of FI messages, but we have no idea if they are better, equal, or inferior to no intervention. This state of affairs is alarming." (p. 276)
Becker (2012) in his critique of Marzano (but relevant for Hattie) states,
"Marzano and his research team had a dependent variable problem. That is, there was no single, comparable measure of 'student achievement' (his stated outcome of interest) that they could use as a dependent variable across all participants. I should note that they were forced into this problem by choosing a lazy research design [a meta-analysis]. A tighter, more focused design could have alleviated this problem."
Problem 4. Hattie Combines Studies that have Totally Different Definitions of Influences, i.e., the Apples vs Oranges problem

This is a major problem with all of Hattie's work. Examples include -

Self-Report - Hattie combines peer assessment with self-report. 

Feedback - Hattie combines background music on an assembly line with monetary rewards, with feedback to teachers and feedback to students.

Snook et al. (2009) was one of the earliest critiques of Hattie's Visible Learning (VL).

Hattie responded to some of their critiques, however, Snook et al. (2010) replied that they were surprised Hattie did not respond to what they consider to be the major problem with Visible Learning, i.e., the lack of consistency in defining variables & carefully defined concepts. They give this example,
'In education, however, the variables being studied are often poorly conceptualised and the studies often far from rigorous. How does one clearly distinguish for research purposes between a classroom that is "teacher centred" and one which is "student centred"' (p. 96 )
Yelle et al. (2016) also summarise the problem with Hattie's combining of studies,
"In education, if a researcher distinguishes, for example, project-based teaching, co-operative work and teamwork, while other researchers do not distinguish or delimit them otherwise, comparing these results will be difficult. It will also be difficult to locate and rigorously filter the results that must be included (or not included) in the meta-analysis. Finally, it will be impossible to know what the averages would be. 
It is therefore necessary to define theoretically the main concepts under study and to ensure that precise and unambiguous criteria for inclusion and exclusion are established. The same thing happens when you try to understand how the author chose the studies on e.g., problem-based learning. The word we find is general, because it compiles a large number of researches, dealing with different school subjects. It should be noted that Hattie notes variances between the different school subjects, which calls for even greater circumspection in the evaluation of the indicators attributed to the different approaches. 
This is why it is crucial to know from which criteria Hattie chose and classified the meta-analyses retained and how they were constituted. How do the authors of the 800 meta-analyses compiled in Hattie (2009) define, for example, the different approaches by problem? In other words, what are the labels that they attach to the concepts they mobilize?
As for the concepts of desirability and efficiency from which these approaches must be located, they themselves are marked by epistemological and ideological issues. What do they mean? According to what types of knowledge is a method desirable? In what way is it effective? What does it achieve?
Hattie's book does not contain information on these important factors, or when it does, it does so too broadly. This vagueness prevents readers from judging for themselves the stability of so-called important variables, their variance or the criteria and methods of their selection. The lack of clarity in the criteria used for the selection of studies is therefore a problem."
Pant (2014) is also critical of Hattie aggregating a wide variety of interventions under one label - 
"which calls into question the theoretical relevance of the analysis." (p. 85)
A great example of this is in the studies on class size.

A comparison of the studies shows different definitions for small and normal classes, e.g. one study defines 23 as a small class but another study defines 23 as a normal class. So comparing the ES is not comparing the same thing!

Schulmeister & Loviscach (2014),
"Even where he has grouped meta-analyses correctly by their independent variables such as instructional interventions, Hattie has in many cases mixed apples and oranges concerning the dependent variables. In some groupings, however, both the independent and the dependent variables do not match easily. For instance, in the group 'feedback', a meta-analysis using music to reinforce behavior is grouped with other studies using instructional interventions that are intended to elicit effects on cognitive processes...
Many of the meta-analyses do not really match the same effect group (i.e., the influence) in which Hattie refers to them. For instance, in the group 'feedback', studies investigating the effect of student feedback on teachers are mixed with studies that examine the effect of teacher feedback on students."
Nielsen & Klitmøller (2017) discuss in detail the many problems of different definitions of feedback and large versus small class sizes - see feedback.

Sundar & Agarwal (2021) are also skeptical of Hattie's work,
"Look for consistency in learning strategies and research methods. If learning strategies included in the meta-analysis are not consistent and logical, then beware! For example, if you find a meta-analysis that groups together “feedback” strategies including teacher praise, computer instruction, oral negative feedback, timing of feedback, and music as reinforcement, does that sound consistent to you?" (p. 6)
Blatchford (2016b) also raises this issue about Hattie,
"it is odd that so much weight is attached to studies that don't directly address the topic on which the conclusions are made" (p. 13).

Hattie's Defense of Apples vs Oranges

Hattie's defense in Visible Learning (VL) was, 

"A common criticism is that it combines 'apples with oranges' and such combining of many seemingly disparate studies is fraught with difficulties. It is the case, however, that in the study of fruit nothing else is sensible" (p. 10).

In his latest defense, Hattie & Hamilton (2020), Real Gold Vs Fool's Gold, Hattie continues with this "fruit" response and once again does not address the significant issues of disparate studies,

"Any literature review involves making balanced judgements about diverse studies. A major reason for the development of meta- analysis was to find a more systematic way to join studies, in a similar way that apples and oranges can make fruit salad. Meta-analysis can be considered to ask about “fruit” and then assess the implications of combining apples and oranges, and the appropriate weighting of this combination. Unlike traditional reviews, meta-analyses provide systematic methods to evaluate the quality of combinations, allow for evaluation of various moderators, and provide excellent data for others to replicate or recombine the results. The key in all cases is the quality of the interpretation of the combined analyses. Further, as noted above, the individual studies can be evaluated for methodological quality." (p. 3-4)
UPDATE 

Finally, Hattie has admitted that these differences, or heterogeneity, is a major problem. In Wisniewski, Zierer & Hattie (2020), The Power of Feedback Revisited,
"...the significant heterogeneity in the data shows that feedback cannot be understood as a single consistent form of treatment." (p. 1)

Problem 5. Effect Size Calculation Can Vary Significantly Depending on the Standard Deviation (SD) chosen.

The 3 common ES calculations are, Cohen's d, Hedges' and Glass’s Δ. The difference is in the SD they use.

Cohen uses the pooled SD, Hedges also uses the pooled SD but adjusts for sample size and Glass uses the control group SD.

Gene Glass the inventor of the meta-analysis in this seminal paper, warned of SD problems, Integrating Findings: The Meta-Analysis of Research (1977). 

Glass shows that since the ES is calculated by dividing by the standard deviation (see formulas above) the standard deviation that is chosen can change the ES in a significant way!

Glass gives this example (p. 370):
"The definition of ES appears uncomplicated, but heterogeneous group variances cause substantial difficulties. Suppose that experimental and control groups have means and standard deviations as follows:
The measure of experimental effect could be calculated either by use of Se or Sc or some combination of the two, such as an average or the square root of the average of their squares or whatever. The differences in ES ensuing from such choices are huge:
The third basis of standardization—the average standard deviation—probably should be eliminated as merely a mindless statistical reaction to a perplexing choice. It must be acknowledged that both the remaining 1.00 and 0.20 are correct; neither can be ruled out as false... However, the control group mean is only one-fifth standard deviation below the mean of the experimental group when measured in control group standard deviations; thus, the average experimental group subject exceeds 58 percent of the subjects in the control group. These facts are neither contradictory nor inconsistent; rather they are two distinct features of a finding which cannot be captured by one number."
Note: A few years after Gene Glass wrote this Cohen (1988) added another method to calculate standard deviation - the 'pooled standard deviation' which averages the variances first and then finds the standard deviation. This seems to be the accepted method now and using this would get d = 0.39.

This illustration demonstrates the variability in effect size (ES) calculations for identical data. Remarkably, the same dataset can yield vastly different effect sizes:

0.20
0.33
0.39
1.00

This range of results underscores the critical impact that different calculation methods can have on effect size estimates, highlighting a significant challenge in comparing and interpreting meta-analytic findings.

If comparing ES across studies, as Hattie does, then Gene Glass warns,
"If some attempt is not made to deal with this problem, a source of inexplicable and annoying variance will be left in a group of effect-size measures" (p. 372).
Hattie references this seminal paper from Glass in VL, but once again ignores the problem.

As a general rule, older studies used Glass’s Δwhile newer studies used Cohen's d or Hedges' g. Note the huge WWC use Hedges' g.

For example, in the studies Hattie used for feedback, there was a range of standard deviations used. Standley (1996, p. 109) used Glass’s Δ while the other studies used Cohen's d.
"The effect sizes of experimental results in this analysis were estimated by contrasting the means of experimental/treatment conditions (Exp) and control/base-line conditions (Con) divided by the standard deviation (SD) of the control/baseline condition, as in the formula below:"
In Problem Based Learning, Gijbels et al. (2005) use Glass’s Δ.

Topphol (2011) also discusses a slight variation of this problem in Hattie's work.
"...in these two cases, the difference between the mean values ​​is the same, D = 20 point. The distribution to the control group is drawn with a solid curve while dotted curve is used for the treatment group. Standard deviations are different, 5 to the left and 17 to the right. This gives different effect sizes, d = 4 and d = 1.18." (p. 464)


Sampling students from small or abnormal populations:

What Topphol displays above is a well-known issue for meta-analyses for several reasons: ES is erroneously larger (due to a smaller standard deviation) and moderating variables are exacerbated.

Using such samples makes it invalid to generalize influences to the broader student population.

Dylan Wiliam explains:



Simpson (2017) also details this problem,
"Researchers can make legitimate design decisions which alter the standard deviation and thus report very different effect sizes for identical interventions. One such design decision is range restriction" (p. 456).
Simpson then insightfully explains that sampling from smaller populations is a major reason why effects for influences such as feedback, meta-cognition, etc., are high while effects for whole school influences - class size, summer school, etc., are low.
"One cannot compare standardised mean differences between sets of studies which tend to use restricted ranges of participants with researcher designed, tightly focussed measures and sets of studies which tend to use a wide range of participants and use standardised tests as measures" (p. 463).
Allerup (2015) also identifies this problem, if one distribution has very little spread, and, moreover, lies entirely within the second sharing outer boundaries then an ES is almost impossible to calculate (p. 6).

Kraft (2019) & Bakker et al. (2019) also confirm this problem.

Yet, Hattie just ignores these issues and uses meta-analyses from abnormal student populations, e.g., ADHD, hyperactivity, emotional/behavioral disturbed, and English Second Language students. 

Also, he uses abnormal subjects from NON-student populations, e.g., doctors, tradesmen, nurses, athletes, sports teams, and military groups.

Professor John O'Neill's (2012b) letter to the NZ Education Minister regarding major issues with Hattie's research. One of the issues he emphasized is Hattie's use of students from abnormal populations.

An example from the research Hattie used is Standley (1996) which Hattie used in Feedback. Standley reported ES up to 35.44 and noted that these were based on small sample sizes (p. 109).

Problem 6. Use of the same data in different meta-analyses:
Shannahan (2017) provides a detailed example,
"What Hattie seems to have done is just take an average of the original effects reported in the various meta-analyses. That sometimes is all right, but it can create a lot of double counting and weighting problems that play havoc with the results. 
For example, Hattie combined two meta-analyses of studies on repeated reading. He indicated that these meta-analyses together included 36 studies. I took a close look myself, and it appears that there were only 35 studies, not 36, but more importantly, four of these studies were double counted. Thus, we have two analyses of 31 studies, not 36, and the effects reported for repeated reading are based on counting four of the studies twice each!
Students who received this intervention outperform those who didn't by 25 percentiles, a sizeable difference in learning. However, because of the double counting, I can't be sure whether this is an over- or underestimate of the actual effects of repeated reading that were found in the studies. Of course, the more meta-analyses that are combined, and the more studies that are double and triple and quadruple counted, the bigger the problem becomes." (p. 751)
Shannahan (2017) provides another detailed example,
"this is (also) evident with Hattie's combination of six vocabulary meta-analyses, each reporting positive learning outcomes from explicit vocabulary teaching. I couldn't find all of the original papers, so I couldn't thoroughly analyze the problems. However, my comparison of only two of the vocabulary meta-analyses revealed 18 studies that weren't there. Hattie claimed that one of the meta-analyses synthesized 33 studies, but it only included 15, and four of those 15 studies were also included in Stahl and Fairbanks's (1986) meta-analysis, whittling these 33 studies down to only 11. One wonders how many more double counts there were in the rest of the vocabulary meta-analyses. 
This problem gets especially egregious when the meta-analyses themselves are counted twice! The National Reading Panel (National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, 2000) reviewed research on several topics, including phonics teaching and phonemic awareness training, finding that teaching phonics and phonemic awareness was beneficial to young readers and to older struggling readers who lacked these particular skills. Later, some of these National Reading Panel meta-analyses were republished, with minor updating, in refereed journals (e.g., Ehri et al., 2001; Ehri, Nunes, Stahl, & Willows, 2002). Hattie managed to count both the originals and the republications and lump them all together under the label Phonics Instruction—ignoring the important distinction between phonemic awareness (chldren's ability to hear and manipulate the sounds within words) and phonics (children's ability to use letter–sound relationships and spelling patterns to read words). That error both double counted 86 studies in the phonics section of Visible Learning and overestimated the amount of research on phonics instruction by more than 100 studies, because the phonemic awareness research is another kettle of fish. Those kinds of errors can only lead educators to believe that there is more evidence than there is and may result in misleading effect estimates." (p. 752)
Wecker et al. (2017) also detail examples,
"In the case of papers summarizing the results of several reviews on the same topic, the problem usually arises that a large part of the primary studies has been included in several of the reviews to be summarized (see Cooper and Koenka 2012 , p. 450 ff.). In the few meta-analyzes available so far, complete meta-analyzes of the first stage have often been ruled out because of overlaps in the primary studies involved (Lipsey and Wilson 1993 , 1197, Peterson 2001 , p.454), as early as overlaps of 25% (Wilson et al Lipsey 2001 , p. 416) or three or more primary studies (Sipe and Curlette 1997, P. 624). 
Hattie, on the other hand, completely ignores the doubts problem despite sometimes significantly greater overlaps. 
For example, on the subject of web-based learning, 14 of the 15 primary studies from the meta-analysis by Olson and Wisher ( 2002 , p. 11), whose mean effect size of 0.24 is significantly different from the results of the other two meta-analyzes on the same topic (0.14 or 0.15), already covered by one of the two other meta-analyzes (Sitzmann et al., 2006 , pp. 654 ff.)" (p. 30)
Kelley & Camilli (2007, p. 25) Many studies use the same data sets. To maintain the statistical independence of the data, only one set of data points from each data set should be included in the meta-analysis.

Hacke (2010),
"Independence is the statistical assumption that groups, samples, or other studies in the meta-analyses are unaffected by each other." (p. 83)
This is a major problem in Hattie's synthesis as many of the meta-analyses that Hattie averages use the same data sets - e.g., much of the same data is used in Teacher Training as is used in Teacher Subject Knowledge.

Hattie finally (+ quietly) admits this is a huge problem in his META-meta Analysis & now promotes the simpler meta-analysis method.

In all of Hattie's defenses, he never addressed this issue of double counting or duplets. Yet, around the same time as his latest published defense, Hattie & Hamilton (2020) "Real Gold vs, Fool's Gold, Hattie was also working on "The Power of Feedback Revisited", Wisniewski, Zierer & Hattie (2020) where they quietly admit this problem & recalculate the ES,
"a source of distortion when using a synthesis approach results from overlapping samples of studies. By integrating a number of meta-analyses dealing with effects of feedback interventions without checking every single primary study, there is a high probability that the samples of primary studies integrated in these meta-analyses are not independent of each other, but at least some primary studies were integrated in more than one meta-analysis. Therefore, these would have to be considered as duplets–primary studies that are included in the result of the synthesis more than once–and consequently cause a distortion. In contrast to meta-synthesis, a meta-analytical approach allows to remove duplets and therefore prevent a distortion of results." (p. 2)

In July 2024, Stephen Vainker highlighted that the issue of data duplication persists in many of Hattie's categories, contrary to Hattie's implication that this problem had been addressed. Vainker's analysis of the Working Memory Training category serves as a striking example. Upon removing significant duplications and off-topic studies, the effect size (ES) for this category dropped dramatically from 0.34 to 0.09.

This substantial reduction raises concerns that the problem of data duplication may be more pervasive than previously acknowledged. It suggests that the reliability of reported effect sizes across various categories in Hattie's work could be compromised. This finding underscores the need for careful scrutiny and potential reassessment of effect sizes in other categories to ensure the accuracy and validity of the meta-analytic results.


Problem 7. Inappropriate Averaging:
Hattie's averaging hides much of the complexity, for example, Snook et al. (2009), on Homework:
"There is also the difficulty which arises amalgamating a large number of disparate studies. When results of many studies are averaged, the complexity of education is ignored: variables such as age, ability, gender, and subject studied are set aside. An example of this problem can be seen in Hattie’s treatment of homework: does homework improve learning or not?

Overall, Hattie finds that the ES of homework is 0.29. Thus a media commentator, reading a summary might justifiably report: “Hattie finds that homework does not make a difference.” When, however, we turn to the section on homework we find that, for example, the ES for elementary (primary in our terms) and high schools students are 0.15 and 0.64 respectively.

Putting it crudely, the figures suggest that homework is very important for high school students but relatively unimportant for primary school students.

There were also significant differences in the effects of homework in mathematics (high effects) and science and social studies (both low effects). Results were high for low ability students and low for high ability students. The nature of the homework set was also influential. (pp 234-236). All these complexities are lost in an average effect size of 0.29" (p. 4).
Schulmeister & Loviscach (2014),
'The effect size given per influence is the mean value of a very broad distribution. For instance, in “Inductive Teaching” Hattie combines two meta-analyses with effect sizes of d = 0.06 and d = 0.59 to a mean effect size of d = 0.33 with a standard error of 0.035. This is like saying ”this six-sided dice does not produce numbers from 1 to 6; rather, it produces the number 3.5 in the mean, and we are pretty sure about the first decimal place of this mean value.”'
Dr. Jim Thornton (2018) Professor of Obstetrics and Gynaecology at Nottingham University said,
"To a medical researcher, it seems bonkers that Hattie combines all studies of the same intervention into a single effect size... In medicine it would be like combining trials of steroids to treat rheumatoid arthritis, effective, with trials of steroids to treat pneumonia, harmful, and concluding that steroids have no effect! I keep expecting someone to tell me I’ve misread Hattie."
Another example from Nilholm (2013) in "It's Time to Critically Review John Hattie on Inductive Teaching",

"Hattie reports two meta-analyzes. One is from 2008 and includes 73 studies related to 'inductive teaching', it shows that the work method generally gives a relatively strong effect. According to a meta-analysis from 1983, which includes 24 studies of inductive teaching in natural sciences, the work method gives a weak effect. 
Hattie simply takes the mean of these two meta-analyzes and thus "inductive teaching" can be dismissed. A more reasonable conclusion would be that "inductive teaching" in science subjects has weak support but that generally it seems to be a good way of' working. Alternatively, it did not appear to work before, but later research gives a much more positive picture" (p. 2).
Nilholm (2013) details another example using "problem-based learning".

This problem is widespread in Hattie's work other examples include class size, feedback, and ability grouping. Also, many of Hattie's researchers warn about averaging:

Mabe and West (1982),
"considerable information would be lost by averaging the often widely discrepant correlations within studies" (p. 291).
Wrigley (2018),
"What now stands proxy for a breadth of evidence is statistical averaging. This mathematical abstraction neglects the contribution of the practitioner’s accumulated experience, a sense of the students’ needs and wishes, and an understanding of social and cultural context" (p. 359).
Wrigley (2018) then goes into detail about inappropriate averaging by Hattie and the EEF, 
"... quite dissimilar studies are thrown together and an aggregate mean of effect sizes calculated. Although some tolerance is acceptable in meta-analysis, since no two research studies are exactly alike, serious problems can arise from aggregating and averaging studies using different definitions of an issue, and based on different curriculum areas, ages and attainment levels of students, types of school, education systems, and so on... 
Indeed, Gene Glass, who originated the idea of meta-analysis, issued this sharp warning about heterogeneity: 'Our biggest challenge is to tame the wild variation in our findings not by decreeing this or that set of standard protocols but by describing and accounting for the variability in our findings. The result of a meta-analysis should never be an average; it should be a graph.' (Robinson, 2004: 29, my italics)" (p. 367).
Wrigley (2018) then quotes Coe,
"One final caveat should be made here about the danger of combining in-commensurable results. Given two (or more) numbers, one can always calculate an average. However, if they are effect sizes from experiments that differ significantly in terms of the outcome measures used, then the result may be totally meaningless...
In comparing (or combining) effect sizes, one should therefore consider carefully whether they relate to the same outcomes... One should also consider whether those outcome measures are derived from the same (or sufficiently similar) instruments and the same (or sufficiently similar) populations... It is also important to compare only like with like in terms of the treatments used to create the differences being measured. In the education literature, the same name is often given to interventions that are actually very different. It could also be that... the actual implementation differed, or that the same treatment may have had different levels of intensity in different studies. In any of these cases, it makes no sense to average out their effects. (Coe, 2002, my italics)" (p. 367)
Sundar & Agarwal (2021) also warn of this problem,
"With each step, the picture becomes fuzzier. At the very start, an individual study will tell you the exact learning strategy, procedure, and student performance. When that study is summarized in a meta-analysis, it is combined with other similar studies. It is useful and reassuring that more than one study found a similar effect for a learning strategy, but less useful than an individual study to learn how to implement it. In sum, a meta-analysis is once removed from the context of an original study.

Now, imagine if we took a meta-analysis and combined it with another meta-analysis. We’ve essentially averaged two averages! Take those two and add even more meta-analyses to the mix. What if all the meta-analyses had different criteria for including studies or focused on different learning outcomes?

None of this is accounted for in a meta-meta-analysis beyond the researchers’ judgement (partly why Hattie’s work is criticized so much). A meta-meta-analysis is now twice removed from an original study. It can offer a broad picture of the state of research, but not much in terms of how to implement a specific strategy in your classroom." (p. 9)
Problem 8. Equal Weightings:

Gene Glass (1977), the inventor of the meta-analysis, whom Hattie quotes regularly, warned of this problem in his seminal paper, Integrating Findings: The Meta-Analysis of Research.
"Precisely what weight to assign to each study in an aggregation is an extremely complex question, one that is not answered adequately by suggestions to pool the raw data (which are rarely available) or to give each study equal weight, regardless of sample size. If one is aggregating arithmetic means, a weighting of results from each study according to SRT(N) might make sense" (p. 358).
Most scholars recommend weighting (Pigott, 2010, p. 9). Larger studies are then weighted greater. If this were done this would affect all the reported ES of Hattie and his rankings would totally change.

The range of student numbers in studies that Hattie used is enormous. In the influence of 'Comprehensive teaching reforms' Hattie cites Borman & D'Agostino (1996) using nearly 42 Million Students! While in the 'gender - attitudes' influence Hattie cites Cooper, Burger & Good (1980) with 219 students. These have equal weight in Hattie's work.

Shannahan (2017) gives more detailed examples,
"when meta-analyses of very different scopes are combined - what if one of the meta-analyses being averaged has many more studies than the others? Simply averaging the results of a meta-analysis based on 1,077 studies with a meta-analysis based on six studies would be very misleading. Hattie combined data from 17 meta-analyses of studies that looked at the effects of students’ prior knowledge or prior achievement levels on later learning. Two of these meta-analyses focused on more than a thousand studies each; others focused on fewer than 50 studies, and one as few as six. Hattie treated them all as equal. Again, potentially misleading." (p. 752)
Pant (2014) verifies Shannahan's analysis and provides another detailed example:
"Hattie (2009) aggregates the mean effect sizes of the original meta-analyzes without weighting them by the number of studies received. Meta-analyzes, which are based on many hundreds of individual studies, enter the d- barometer with the same weight as meta-analyzes with only five primary studies. The consequences of this approach for the content conclusions will be briefly demonstrated by a numerical example from Hattie's (2009) data. The determined from four meta effect of the teaching method of direct instruction (Direct Instruction) is to Hattie (2009 , p 205;) d = 0.59 and thus falls into the 'desired zone' ( d > 0.4). 
Direct instruction is by no means undisputed, highly structured, and teacher-centered teaching. Looking at the processed meta-analyzes one by one, it is striking that the analysis by far the largest in 232 primary studies (Borman et al., 2003 ) is the one with the least effect size (i.e. = 0.21). If the three meta-analyzes for which information on the standard error were presented were weighted according to their primary number of studies (Hill et al. 2007, Shadish and Haddock 2009), the resulting effect size would be d = 0.39 and thus no longer in the 'desired' zone of action defined by Hattie." (p. 95)
Wecker et al. (2017) give an example of using weighted averages:
"This would mean a descent from 26th place to 98th in his ranking." (p. 31)
Blatchford (2016) also warns of this problem,
"unfortunately many reviews and meta-analyses have given them equal weighting" (p. 15).
See (2017) also details the issue of quality of evidence & averaging by Hattie, Marzano, and others, 
"there are studies which involved only one participant, some had no comparator groups and some involved children with specific learning difficulties or had huge attrition as large as 70%. These may form the majority of studies reporting huge positive effects. On the other hand, the few good quality studies may report small effects. 
Averaging effect sizes from across studies of different quality giving equal weights to all can lead to misleading conclusions" (p. 10).  
One of the earliest critiques, Arnold (2011) also identifies this issue,
"I was surprised that Hattie has chosen to summarise the effect sizes of the 800 meta-analyses using unweighted averages. Small and large meta-analyses have equal weight, while I would assume that the number of studies on which a meta-analysis is based indicates its validity and importance. Instead I would have opted for weighted averaging by number of studies, students or effect sizes. At a minimum, it would be interesting to see whether the results are robust to the choice of averaging."
Proulx (2017) and Thibault (2017) also question Hattie's averaging.

Example - Visual Perception Programs-


Hattie's ES is d = 0.55. But if we weigh according to the number of students (with the assumption studies reporting no students are assigned the lowest number of students, 4,400 (highlighted yellow). We get a weighted ES, d = 0.79 shooting this up from #35 to #7.

Nielsen & Klitmøller (2017) also show this problem in their detailed analysis of Hattie's use of feedback studies- see feedback.

In his latest 2020 defense, Real Gold Vs Fool's Gold, Hattie does not address and simply dismisses the detailed issues presented by all the peer reviews above (p. 2),


Yet, around the same time Hattie was working on "The Power of Feedback Revisited", Wisniewski, Zierer & Hattie (2020), reported a reduced ES of 0.48 and comment contrary to Hattie's claim above,
"The average weighted effect size differs considerably from the results of meta-synthesis (d = 0.79, Hattie and Timperley, 2007)." (p. 11)

They then try to explain why there is such a difference in ES and cite many of the issues already detailed on this page,

"A second reason for the difference between synthesis and meta-analysis could be our weighting of the effect sizes by precision which was not applied in the Visible Learning research." (p. 11)

Problem 9. Confounding Variables:

Related to problem 1 - the research designers usually put a lot of thought into the controlling of other variables. Random assignments and double blindness are the major strategies used. Unfortunately, most of the studies Hattie cites, do not use these strategies. This introduces major moderating variables into the study. Class size is a good example, many studies compare the achievement of small versus large classes in schools, but many schools assign lower achieving students to smaller classes, they do not use random assignment.

Thibault (2017) gives other examples (English translation),

"a goal of the mega-analyzes is to relativize the factors of variation that have not been identified in a study, balancing in some so the extreme data influenced by uncontrolled variables. But by combining all the data as well as the particular context that is associated with each study, we eliminate the specificities of each context, which for many give meaning to the study itself! We then lose the richness of the data and the meaning of what we try to measure.
It even happens that brings together results that are deeply different, even contradictory in their nature.
 
For example, the source of the feedback remains risky, as explained by Proulx (2017), given that Hattie (2009) claims to have realized that the feedback comes from the student and not from the teacher, but it is no less certain that his analysis focused on feedback from the teacher. It is right to question this way of doing things since the studies quantitatively seek to control variables to isolate the effect of each. When combining data from different studies, the attempt to control the variables is annihilated. Indeed, all these studies have not necessarily sought to control the same variables in the same way, they have probably used instruments different and carried out with populations difficult to compare. So these combinations are not just uninformative, but they significantly skew the meaning."
Nielsen & Klitmøller (2017) discuss the problem of Hattie not addressing moderating factors, the interaction of factors and the disparate operational definitions of different studies, 
"it is our assessment that in four of the five "heaviest" surveys that mentioned in connection with Hattie's cover of Feedback, it is conceptually unclear whether they are operates with a feedback term that is identical with Hattie's" (p. 11, translated from Danish).
Blichfeldt (2011),
"to validly put more blurred variables into accurate calculations seems problematic...
...he allows a very low degree of precision as to what variables are included in the calculations as to what may be expected and how results can be understood. At the same time, he uses calculations and statistics that should require precision and control that it is hard to find coverage for. Which does not prevent him from producing results as very precise with two decimal places...
What he studies is summarized statistical relationships between unclear variables and skill tests."
Nilholm (2013) confirms this problem,
"Hattie's major failure is to report summative measurements of meta-analysis without taking into account so-called moderating factors. Working methods can work better for a particular subject, a certain grade, some students and so on. Hattie believes that the significance of such moderating factors is less than one can think. I would argue that they are often very noticeable, as in the examples I reported [see problem-based learning and inductive teaching] Unless such moderating factors are taken into account, direct generalizations will be made directly" (p. 3).
Allerup (2015) commenting on Hattie's use of ES, calls for a more sophisticated multivariate analysis,
"it is well known that analyses in the educational world often require the involvement of more dimensional (multivariate) analyses" (p. 8).
Hattie rarely acknowledges this problem now, but in earlier work, Hattie & Clinton (2008) stated:
"student test scores depend on multiple factors, many of which are out of the control of the teacher." (p. 320)
Another pertinent example is from Kulik and Kulik (1992) - see ability grouping:

Two different methods produced distinctly different results. Each of the 11 studies with same-age control groups showed greater achievement average ES in these studies was 0.87.

However, if you use the (usually 1 year older) students as the control group, The average ES in the 12 studies was 0.02. Hattie uses this figure in the category 'ability grouping for gifted students'.

Hattie does not include the d = 0.87. I think a strong argument can be made that the result d = 0.87 should be reported instead of the d = 0.02 as the accelerated students should be compared to the student group they came from (same-age students) rather than the older group they are accelerating into.

The Combination of Influences:

In addition, a study may measure the combination of many influences. For example, using class size, how do you remove other influences from the study? For example, time on task, motivation, behavior, teacher subject knowledge, feedback, home life, welfare, etc.

Nielsen & Klitmøller (2017) discuss this problem in detail.

But, Hattie wavers on this major issue. In his commentary on 'within-class grouping' about Lou et al. (1996, p. 94) Hattie does report some degree of additivity,
"this analysis shows that the effect of grouping depends on class size. In large classes (more than 35 students) the mean effect of grouping is d = 0.35, whereas in small classes (less than 26 students) the mean effect is d = 0.22."
But in Chapter 11 of VL, Hattie states, 
"It is unlikely that many of the effects reported in this book are additive" (VL, p. 256).
Problem 10. Quality of Studies:
"Extraordinary claims require extraordinary evidence." Carl Sagan
Hattie's constant proclamation (VL 2012 summary, p. 3),
"it is the interpretations that are critical, rather than data itself."
This is opposite to the Scientific Method paradigm as Snook et al. (2009, p. 2) explain:
"Hattie says that he is not concerned with the quality of the research... of course, quality is everything. Any meta-analysis that does not exclude poor or inadequate studies is misleading, and potentially damaging if it leads to ill-advised policy developments. He also needs to be sure that restricting his data base to meta-analyses did not lead to the omission of significant studies of the variables he is interested in."
O'Neill (2012a) writes a significant letter to the NZ Education Minister & Hattie regarding the poor quality of Hattie's research, in particular, the overuse of studies about University, graduate or preschool students and the danger of making classroom policy decisions without consulting other forms of evidence, e.g., case and naturalistic studies. 
"The method of the synthesis and, consequently, the rank ordering are highly problematic" (p. 7).
Hattie ignored O'Neill's critique and constantly proclaims,
"Almost all of this data is based on what happens in real schools with real kids..."


See (2017), emphasizes the lack of quality in the evidence by Hattie, 
"there are several problems with relying on such evidence taken from meta-analyses of meta-analyses for policy and practice. 
First, much of it is not particularly robust (small-scale, involving non-randomisation of participants, based on summaries of effects across a wide range of subjects and age groups). 
Second, no consideration was taken of the quality of research in the synthesis of existing evidence. For example, there are studies which involved only one participant, some had no comparative groups and some involved children with specific learning difficulties or had huge attrition as large as 70%. These may form the majority of studies reporting huge positive effects. On the other hand, the few good quality studies may report small effects. Averaging effect sizes from across studies of different quality giving equal weights to all can lead to misleading conclusions" (p. 10). 
Schulmeister & Loviscach (2014),
"Many of the meta-analyses used by Hattie are dubious in terms of methodology. Hattie obviously did not look into the individual empirical studies that form the bases of the meta-analyses, but used the latter in good faith."
Nielsen & Klitmøller (2017) also discuss the problems of quality using examples from VL, p. 75 and 196 - see feedback.
"Hattie does not deal with the potential problems in his own investigation but instead refers to others who have to deal with problems in connection with meta-analyses generally. In other words, Hattie is not directly concerned about the quality of his own investigation. 
In some selected contexts nevertheless, Hattie does throw out studies based on quality, but this neither consistent nor systematic" (translated from Danish, p. 10).
Nielsen & Klitmøller's criticism is based on Hattie sometimes using the following protocols to justify the exclusion of meta-analyses,
"mainly based on doctoral dissertations... with mostly attitudinal outcomes, many were based on adult samples... and some of the sample sizes were tiny" (VL, p. 196).
Lind (2013) confirms this and also uses more examples from VL, pp. 196 ff. Where, he accused Hattie of disregarding studies that did not suit him, e.g. kinesthetic learning.

The Encyclopedia of Measurement and Statistics outlines the problem of quality: 
"many experts agree that a useful research synthesis should be based on findings from high-quality studies with methodological rigour. Relaxed inclusion standards for studies in a meta-analysis may lead to a problem that Hans J. Eysenck in 1978 labelled as garbage in, garbage out."
In modern termsDr. Gary Smith (2014),
"garbage in, gospel out." (p. 25)
Most researchers that Hattie used warn about the quality of studies, e.g., Slavin (1990)
"any measure of central tendency in a meta-analysis... should be interpreted in light of the quality and consistency of the studies from which it was derived, not as a finding in its own right. 
'best evidence synthesis' of any education policy should encourage decision makers to favour results from studies with high internal and external validity—that is, randomised field trials involving large numbers of students, schools, and districts." (p. 477)
Janson (2018),
"Hattie compiles large numbers of meta-analyses of all kinds for his meta-meta-analyses, without paying too much attention to the meaning or quality of the original studies."
The U.S. Department of Education has set up the National Center for Education Research whose focus is to investigate the quality of educational research. Their results are published in the What Works Clearing House. They also publish a Teacher Practice Guide which differs markedly from Hattie's results - see Other Researchers.

Importantly they focus on the QUALITY of the research and reserve their highest ratings for research that uses randomised division of students into a control and an experimental group. Where students are non-randomly divided into a control and experimental group for what they term a quasi-experiment, a moderate rating is used. However, the two groups must have some sort of equivalence measure before the intervention. A low rating is used for other research design methods - e.g., correlation studies.

However, once again, Hattie ignores these issues and makes an astonishing caveat, there is, 
"no reason to throw out studies automatically because of lower quality" (p. 11).
Problem 11. Time over which each study ran:

Given Hattie interprets an ES of 0.40 as equivalent to 1 year of schooling, and his polemic related to this figure:
"I would go further and claim that those students who do not achieve at least a 0.40 improvement in a year are going backwards..." (p. 250).
In terms of teacher performance, he takes this one step further by declaring teachers who don't attain up to an ES of 0.40 are 'below average'. Hattie (2010, p. 87).

This means, as Dylan Wiliam points out, that studies need to be controlled for the time over which they run, otherwise legitimate comparisons cannot be made.

Wiliam, who also produced the seminal research, Inside the black box, also reflects on his own research and cautions,
"it is only within the last few years that I have become aware of just how many problems there are. Many published studies on feedback, for example, are conducted by psychology professors, on their own students, in experimental sessions that last a single day. The generalizability of such studies to school classrooms is highly questionable. 
In retrospect, therefore, it may well have been a mistake to use effect sizes in our booklet 'Inside the black box' to indicate the sorts of impact that formative assessment might have.

I do still think that effect sizes are useful... If the effect sizes are based on experiments of similar duration, on similar populations, using outcome measures that are similar in their sensitivity to the effects of teaching, then I think comparisons are reasonable. Otherwise, I think effect sizes are extremely difficult to interpret."
Hattie (2015) finally admitted this was an issue:
"Yes, the time over which any intervention is conducted can matter (we find that calculations over less than 10-12 weeks can be unstable, the time is too short to engender change, and you end up doing too much assessment relative to teaching). These are critical moderators of the overall effect-sizes and any use of hinge=0.4 should, of course, take these into account."
Yet, for a long time this has not affected his public pronouncements nor additions or reductions of studies to his database. 

NOTE: in his 2020 update, The Power of Feedback Revisited, Hattie quietly agreed with the previous peer review critique & finally removed MOST of the original 23 studies he used for Feedback and deduced a very reduced ES of 0.48.

However, he has not made any adjustments according to Wiliam's critique, that the time over which the study runs needs to be taken into account, see A Years Progress.

These issues have been known for a long time and many researchers, e.g., Berk (2011), recommend a focus on high-quality INDIVIDUAL studies (as does the What Works Clearing House), 
"One should applaud the view that public policy is to be based on evidence. However, what qualifies as evidence, let alone strong evidence, is too often left unspecified. Into this vacuum has been drawn a mix of evaluations ranging from excellent to terrible. 
...the importance of meta-analysis for estimating causal effects has been grossly overrated. A conventional literature review will often do better. At the very least, readers will not be swayed by statistical malpractice disguised as statistical razzle-dazzle" (p. 199). 

 

8 comments:

  1. What do you do if you are using data with no definitive parameters? Our major assessment is NWEA Map and students do not score a 0-100 result and there isn't really a cap they could attain. It appears these calculations result in skewed effect sizes. Is there a solution to this?

    ReplyDelete
  2. the students don't have to score between 0-100. My example just used a score out of 100 for simplicity. You can use any test with any total score as long as the two groups of students or the before and after scores are from the same test.

    In your case, it looks as though NWEA compare each individual student with a normed result, so effect size = (student score - normed score)/ divided by pooled standard deviation.

    Another slightly different use of effect sizes, is the USA benchmark Effect sizes for their standardised tests - see here & scroll down - https://visablelearning.blogspot.com/p/a-years-progress.html

    and here's an example of students who did a test out of 10 marks -

    https://docs.google.com/spreadsheets/d/1R8EkDv6MFQ8UbpN1T79EVR17xVhGuz-cFXDo0aquS24/edit?usp=sharing

    ReplyDelete
  3. “Hattie calls the first equation the Random Method, which is the same as Clark & Mayer (2016) above, and the second the Fixed Method (VL, p. 8).”

    I can't find this claim in VL. I think Random Method and Fixed Method do not refer to the equations

    ReplyDelete
    Replies
    1. Hattie lists the 2 equations on p. 8, but I thought he implied they are the 2 methods on p. 12. You are correct the Random and Fixed methods do not refer to those equations.

      Delete
  4. I am still confused about the Random Method and Fixed Method. I think they refer to random-effects models and fixed-effects models that the original meta-analysis used. I can find some explanation about these two models on google. It seems that:

    The fixed-effects models assume that all studies included in a meta-analysis share a common effect size, rather than they are using a pre and post-test comparison design as the equation shows.

    Likewise, the random-effects models assume that studies included in a meta-analysis are heterogeneous, rather than they are using RCTs design.

    or they are truly relative to the experiment design?

    ReplyDelete
    Replies
    1. Please check the follow article. I can't find any clue that fixed or random effects models are referring to experiment design.

      Fixed or random effects meta-analysis? Common methodological issues in systematic reviews of effectiveness https://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.1049.153&rep=rep1&type=pdf

      Delete
    2. thank you for your clarification, you are correct the 2 equations just refer to 2 different ways to calculate the Effect Size, they do not refer to the Fixed or Random Models.

      Delete
  5. Conversion of correlation to effect size method

    ReplyDelete